Ask a Librarian

Threre are lots of ways to contact a librarian. Choose what works best for you.

HOURS TODAY

Closed

Reference Desk

CONTACT US BY PHONE

(802) 656-2022

Voice

(802) 503-1703

Text

MAKE AN APPOINTMENT OR EMAIL A QUESTION

Schedule an Appointment

Meet with a librarian or subject specialist for in-depth help.

Email a Librarian

Submit a question for reply by e-mail.

WANT TO TALK TO SOMEONE RIGHT AWAY?

Library Hours for Saturday, November 23rd

All of the hours for today can be found below. We look forward to seeing you in the library.
HOURS TODAY
Closed
MAIN LIBRARY

SEE ALL LIBRARY HOURS
WITHIN HOWE LIBRARY

MapsM-Th by appointment, email govdocs@uvm.edu

Media ServicesClosed

Reference DeskClosed

OTHER DEPARTMENTS

Special CollectionsClosed

Dana Health Sciences Library10:00 am - 6:00 pm

 

CATQuest

Search the UVM Libraries' collections

UVM Theses and Dissertations

Browse by Department
Format:
Online
Author:
Broomer, Matthew Cyril
Dept./Program:
Psychology
Year:
2021
Degree:
M.A.
Abstract:
In instrumental learning, extensive training of a response typically generates a habit. The transition from goal-directed to habitual behavioral control is often considered unidirectional; however, recent evidence suggests this is not the case. Under certain conditions, goal-directed control may be restored to a behavior that was previously habitual. We identify the infralimbic cortex (IL) as a participant in this process and draw on both instrumental learning and fear extinction literatures to further our understanding of IL function. Four experiments explored the role of IL in flexible behavior. Experiments 1a and 1b were instrumental learning experiments. Experiment 1a used a previously documented procedure to establish a single instrumental response as goal-directed in one context and habitual in another. Following reversible pharmacological inactivation of IL, goal-directed action was converted to habit, a novel finding. Experiment 1b expanded on this result by examining IL inactivation in an instrumental response that had received only minimal training in a single context. Here, IL inactivation had no effect, suggesting that IL is brought online by a multi-stage training history (i.e., action to habit). Experiments 2a and 2b were fear conditioning experiments. Given the substantial literature regarding IL involvement in fear extinction, these experiments were intended to confirm the efficacy of our IL manipulation in the same rats from Experiments 1a and 1b, respectively. In both experiments, rats underwent contextual fear conditioning followed by two extinction sessions. Rats were then tested under IL inactivation either following extinction or following a subsequent reacquisition session in which the extinguished context was associated with shock again. In both experiments, IL inactivation impaired the expression of extinction learning. Although IL inactivation did not significantly affect the expression of reacquisition, the effect of IL inactivation depended on whether it was tested following extinction or reacquisition. These results sketch the foundation of a framework in which IL is required to switch between available behavioral states when two conflicting things have been learned. The view may integrate the separately-studied roles of IL in instrumental habit learning and fear extinction learning.